A synthesizing mind A memoir from the creator of multiple intelligences theory

Howard Gardner, 1943-

Book - 2020

"Gardner's memoir places his work on multiple intelligences within the arc of his academic career, and presents a defense of the scholarly and public value of powerful 'works of synthesis.'"--

Saved in:
This item has been withdrawn.

2nd Floor Show me where

150.92/Gardner
All copies withdrawn
Location Call Number   Status
2nd Floor 150.92/Gardner Withdrawn
Subjects
Genres
Autobiographies
Published
Cambridge, Massachusetts : The MIT Press [2020]
Language
English
Main Author
Howard Gardner, 1943- (author)
Physical Description
xv, 258 pages, 28 unnumbered pages of plates : illustrations ; 21 cm
Bibliography
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN
9780262044264
  • Introduction
  • Part I. The Formation of a Synthesizing Mind
  • 1. My Ten-Year-Old Mind
  • 2. Weighing Tests and Tracks
  • 3. Discovering Enduring Interests and Alluring Approaches
  • 4. Resisting a Single Frame
  • 5. Assembling a Network of Enterprises
  • Part II. Multiple Intelligences: Reframing a Human Conversation
  • 6. Steps toward Frames
  • 7. Reactions to MI
  • 8. MI in the World
  • 9. Putting MI in Its Place
  • Part III. Unpacking the Synthesizing Mind
  • 10. On to Education and Good Work
  • 11. My Scattered Pursuits and the Mind That Connects Them
  • 12. Understanding the Synthesizing Mind
  • Acknowledgments
  • Appendix: List of Biog Posts by the Author
  • Bibliography
  • Index
Review by Kirkus Book Review

The latest view of intelligence combined with a compelling autobiography. Gardner, professor of cognition and education at Harvard Graduate School of Education, has made groundbreaking contributions to cognitive psychology, and this lively memoir includes an extensive yet accessible introduction to his work. The son of refugees from Nazi Germany, he was a bright, curious child with enough musical talent to teach piano. Breezing through Harvard, he sampled the humanities, but psychoanalyst Erik Erikson piqued his interest in the study of human intellectual development. After these early life details, the author delivers a lucid account of the life of a successful academic: thinking, investigating, teaching, and arguing about unanswered questions and then communicating his ideas in hundreds of blog posts, articles, and several dozen books, many for a popular audience. Dismissing the controversy over whether psychology is a "hard" science, Gardner explains that he avoids laboratory experiments, preferring to examine existing ideas to see where they lead. Possessing a "synthesizing mind," he prefers to "take in a lot of information, reflect on it, and then organize it in a way that is useful." Although not shy about describing other contributions, his fame rests on theories of how humans process information. Unhappy with the standard measure, the IQ test, which stresses language and logic, Gardner absorbed the massive literature on cognitive psychology and concluded that humans possess seven distinct techniques for acquiring knowledge, which he called "intelligences." Besides the two IQ standards, he added musical, spatial (navigation, chess playing), kinesthetic (athletics, dancing), interpersonal (leadership, salesmanship), and intrapersonal (self-knowledge, wisdom), which he introduced in his 1983 book, Frames of Mind. He later added several more. Gardner admits that he "would not have achieved a certain degree of notoriety if I had chosen some other noun: seven capacities; or seven competences; or seven kinds of minds" or talents, gifts, or learning styles. "Intelligence" caught everyone's attention. An insightful memoir from an eminent psychologist. Copyright (c) Kirkus Reviews, used with permission.

Copyright (c) Kirkus Reviews, used with permission.

We can't do the experiment, but I am convinced that I would not be writing this memoir, and would not have achieved a certain degree of notoriety if I had chosen some other noun: seven capacities; or seven competences; or seven kinds of minds, recalling the book that I had outlined some years earlier; or seven talents or seven gifts, or even, to use a phrase I detest, seven learning styles. No, with little doubt, it was by virtue of the decision to beg, borrow, seize, or steal the word intelligence that I caught the attention of so many readers, including the then all powerful members of the so-called chattering classes. Ever since Alfred Binet (1857-1911), a great French psychologist, the use of the word "intelligence" and the instrument called the "intelligence quotient test" or the "IQ test" has taken on a special, even sacred meaning in much of the Western world. We want to be intelligent ourselves; we want to know and befriend people who are intelligent; and above all, we want our children to be intelligent. How best to convey that capacity to the world? Take a short-answer, multiple-choice style test, tote up the right and wrong answers, and compare your performance with that of peers. If you do about as well as others in your cohort, you'll be called average. If you do better, say by a standard deviation, you'll be called smart; and if you don't do as well as your peers, you'll be called dull, dumb, or, to be more politically correct, a bit on the slow side or "very nice." And if you are two or three standard deviations away from the mean of the group, you'll be hailed as a "genius" at one end of the bell curve or dismissed as an "imbecile" or an "idiot" at the other, less happy end of the curve. The devising and administration of the intelligence tests turns out to be a notable achievement of psychological measurement, but also a perilous one. As a scholar, having settled on a noun, I also needed a definition of what constituted an intelligence. Over the years, I have tinkered with the exact wording of that definition. But in Frames of Mind , first published in 1983, I put forth my thinking in informal terms: "A human intellectual competence must entail a set of skills of problem solving--enabling the individual to resolve genuine problems or difficulties that he or she encounters, and, when appropriate, to create an effective product--and must entail the potential for finding or creating problems--hereby laying the groundwork for the acquisition of new knowledge" (62). Looking back nearly four decades, I am relieved that I was appropriately modest in my claims for the definition and the criteria of an intelligence: "Ultimately it would certainly be desirable to have an algorithm for the selection of an intelligence . . . at present, however, it must be admitted that the selection (or rejection) of a candidate intelligence is reminiscent more of an artistic judgment than of a scientific assessment . . . where my procedure does take a scientific turn is in the making public of the grounds for the judgment, so that other investigators can review the evidence and draw their own conclusions" ( Frames of Mind , 63). I had thrown down the gauntlet: Goodbye, or even good riddance, to a single intelligence as probed by the ubiquitous IQ test; greetings to multiple intelligences, modes of assessment yet to be determined. I would soon see how the world would react to the words and claims in the new and perhaps daring synthesis that I was putting forth. Excerpted from A Synthesizing Mind: A Memoir from the Creator of Multiple Intelligences Theory by Howard Gardner All rights reserved by the original copyright owners. Excerpts are provided for display purposes only and may not be reproduced, reprinted or distributed without the written permission of the publisher.